Anxious mothers... anxious babies?
نویسنده
چکیده
Many mothers of infants experience days of worry and weeks of work—an experience made so much harder by the presence of a crying infant. Up to 20% of mothers in developed countries report excessive crying in their infants. Although often thought of as a benign, self-limiting condition, infant crying—in particular, crying that lasts more than 3 h per day, for more than 3 days per week and for more than 3 weeks in a row—is associated with child abuse, early weaning and maternal depression. The cause(s) of excessive infant crying remains elusive. There is no doubt, however, that a mother’s mood and caregiving style can impact directly on an infant’s behaviour and vice versa. It is well recognised that mothers who experience postnatal depression may withdraw from their infant who, in turn, may withdraw from their mother. It is also well recognised that mothers of infants with excessive crying are more likely to report high levels of depression symptoms than mothers of infants without such crying. However, the role of maternal anxiety in infant crying has not been well researched. The article by Petzoldt et al shines a light on this largely neglected area. The authors highlight the unique contribution of maternal anxiety to the development of excessive crying by following a community sample of 306 mothers across seven time points from early pregnancy to 16 months postpartum. They found that mothers with an anxiety disorder prior to pregnancy were at higher risk of having a child with excessive crying at 2, 4 or 16 months postpartum compared with mothers without an anxiety disorder, and that the risk increased further for mothers who developed an anxiety disorder during pregnancy. They hypothesised that this may be due to parenting style, that is, maternal anxiety leading to intrusive parenting in turn increasing infant crying. This is a scenario often seen in the consultation room where an anxious mother tries to soothe her crying baby by constant rocking and jiggling at a time when her baby is actually tired and needs help to be settled calmly to sleep. This intrusive parenting and misreading of the baby’s tired cues intensifies the baby’s crying. Beyond the first year (ie, around 16 months), excessive crying may reflect a child’s inability to selfsoothe and self-regulate. This could again be exacerbated by the mother’s parenting. An anxious mother is more likely to be overinvolved and overprotective, stepping in to soothe her toddler rather than allowing him opportunities to learn to do so himself. Future research should explore whether parenting styles mediate the relationship between maternal perinatal anxiety and infant/toddler crying and the potential for interventions around this. Another explanation for the relationship between perinatal anxiety and infant crying is the foetal programming hypothesis. Maternal anxiety during pregnancy can lead to increased maternal cortisol crossing the placenta. This may affect the development of the fetus’s hypothalamic-pituitary-axis, limbic system and prefrontal cortex, leading to an infant who is more irritable and less able to self-regulate. A further explanation may be that a genetic predisposition to anxiety is inherited by her infant from the mother. Persistent infant crying is a risk factor for later child internalising disorders such as anxiety. Thus, the relationship between maternal anxiety and infant crying is probably due to a complex interplay of genetics, foetal programming and parenting. Parenting by fathers should not be forgotten. Fathers play an increasing role in the care of their infants and can also experience anxiety disorders. Paternal antenatal and postnatal depression have been shown to uniquely contribute to later child behaviour problems, and paternal anxiety may play a similar role in infant crying. Future research should examine this. Surprisingly, maternal depressive disorders experienced either before or during pregnancy did not predict maternal report of excessive infant crying. The reasons for this are unclear. It may be, as the authors suggest, that maternal depression leads to maternal withdrawal, which discourages infant crying. It may also be that mothers who are depressed are less attuned to their infant and are not aware of the extent of their infant’s crying. Petzoldt et al’s study had a number of strengths. It followed a large, prospective cohort with excellent retention and used validated diagnostic interviews to ascertain maternal mental health disorders. However, as in most community-based research, women from lower socioeconomic groups were under-represented as were women from different cultural backgrounds. Whether the author’s findings hold true for these groups remains to be seen. There are some potential clinical implications of this study. The authors suggest that mothers could be potentially screened for anxiety in the perinatal period. They are rightly cautious in this suggestion as a number of criteria for screening programmes have not been met. For example, we do not have a clear understanding of the natural history of maternal perinatal anxiety disorders; we do not have a ‘gold standard’ screening test that is brief, valid and acceptable; we do not have data to tell us when a screen should be done during the course of pregnancy and we do not know who should treat a woman with perinatal anxiety nor how best to do so. Thus, the study by Petzoldt et al opens up a wealth of research opportunities in this complex landscape. Future research needs to replicate these findings in perinatal cohorts around the world. The roles of parenting styles, genetics the intrauterine environment, and fathers’ mental health and parenting need to be further explored. In the interim, health professionals who care for women and their infants could open up a conversation around maternal anxiety and its perceived impact on the mother’s parenting styles and the infant’s response, provided professional support is available to address any issues arising. This needs to be done sensitively as the last thing we need to do is add to a mother’s ‘day of worry’ by blaming her for her infant’s crying.
منابع مشابه
بررسی تأثیر مراقبت کانگورویی بر تنش مادران نوزادان بستری در بخش مراقبتهای ویژه نوزادان
Background & Aim: Mothers often feel anxious about giving care to their premature babies. This maternal stress is associated with negative attitudes and behaviors towards babies. The aim of this study was to assess the effect of Kangaroo mother care on stress among mothers with premature babies . Methods & Materials: This quasi-experimental study was carried out on 46 pairs of mothers an...
متن کاملAnxiety Level between Mothers of Premature- Born Babies and Those of Normal Born Babies
This study was designed to find out the anxiety levels of the mothers in relation to birth status of the babies and anxiety in relation to age of the mothers. The sample consisted mothers of 25 premature born babies and mothers of 25 normal born babies. They were given by the Bengali version of “Dutt Personality Inventory” to answer. Data were analyzed by t-test, chi-square and product moment c...
متن کاملDoll play narratives about starting school in children of socially anxious mothers, and their relation to subsequent child school-based anxiety.
Child social anxiety is common, and predicts later emotional and academic impairment. Offspring of socially anxious mothers are at increased risk. It is important to establish whether individual vulnerability to disorder can be identified in young children. The responses of 4.5 year-old children of mothers with social phobia (N = 62) and non-anxious mothers (N = 60) were compared, two months be...
متن کاملParent-Child Interactions With Anxious Children and With Their Siblings: An Observational Study
In the present study, parent-child interactions with anxious children were compared to parent-child interactions with the anxious children's nonsymptomatic siblings and parent-child interactions with nonclinic children. Participants included 33 anxious children, their parents and siblings, and 14 nonclinic childreh and their parents. Parent-child interactions were observed during two discus, si...
متن کاملChildren's interpretation and avoidant response biases in response to non-salient and salient situations: relationships with mothers' threat perception and coping expectations.
This study examined the effect of situation salience on interpretation and avoidant response biases in clinically anxious and non-clinical children. The relationship between mothers' threat perception and expectations of their children's coping, and children's threat perception and coping expectations was also assessed. Forty clinically anxious and 40 non-clinical children (ages 7-14) participa...
متن کاملMaternal anxiety, mother-infant interactions, and infants' response to challenge.
Children of anxious mothers are at risk for social-emotional difficulties and disturbed, early interactions with their mother may account for some of the risk. This study evaluated the association between maternal anxiety, features of mother-infant interactions, and infants' emotion regulation during stressful situations (still-face, play with a stranger). Thirty-four anxiety-disordered mothers...
متن کاملذخیره در منابع من
با ذخیره ی این منبع در منابع من، دسترسی به آن را برای استفاده های بعدی آسان تر کنید
برای دانلود متن کامل این مقاله و بیش از 32 میلیون مقاله دیگر ابتدا ثبت نام کنید
ثبت ناماگر عضو سایت هستید لطفا وارد حساب کاربری خود شوید
ورودعنوان ژورنال:
- Archives of disease in childhood
دوره 99 9 شماره
صفحات -
تاریخ انتشار 2014